Wednesday, November 26, 2014

The Joke Was on Me: I Became a Schizophrenic

So my brother fell and bumped his nose and our father caught me laughing (at age ten.) He said, "Don't take pleasure in the misfortune of others or you'll have some misfortune of your own."

I said, "Yea. Right, Dad," and kept laughing.

At twenty I turned into a paranoid schizophrenic. (We do not have more than one personality and we are not abnormally violent (when we accept psychiatric treatment), despite the commonplace pseudo-intellectual comparison of schizophrenia with split personality, despite the comedians who think they're so funny, despite all the blockbuster movies: "Psycho," for instance, despite the local eyewitness corporatist "if it bleeds it leads" sensationalized hyper-vigilant false libelous news about us, and despite the thoughtless, hurtful, everyday words people use unconscionably: "psycho, loony, madman, schizo," because nobody ever tells them it's wrong, but they know it's wrong to say "nigger, kike, spic," and the like about others who haven't lost their minds. Aristotle said the ability to reason is what sets humans apart from other animals. I suppose then that schizophrenics are not persons, by that old-fashioned rhetoric.)

It wasn't until after I turned 55 that I read the work of the autistic self-advocates and learned to be proud of the grandiose portion of my delusional disorder. My effort to help save the world along the lines of B.F. Skinner's scientific design of society would not be possible if I were not so big-headed. (Not that it's about me, either. I usually keep my name down from the internet and I get no pay for any of it.)

I recommend the following to my readers and followers: Don't laugh at a schizophrenic or you'll have some bad luck as well.

Thursday, October 23, 2014

A Self-Controlled, Return-to-Baseline, Functional Analysis of the Human Foot as a Cleaning Apparatus

Wiki-Commons
(Author's note: This satire is an unfinished draft. I hit the darned "publish" button too soon and promoted it in social media. That was a big mistake and I don't want to take it down. This would lead social media followers to a dead link. It is still readable, but it contains flaws. I'm juggling a few ongoing projects at the moment. Come back again for newer version of this post. Same bat time. Same bat channel. Dave Jersey. Nov. 20, 2014)

Abstract

This study confirmed the hypothesis that multitudinous barefoot-to-floor cleaning trials during elongated habitation periods do noticeably clean a varnished wooden floor. The cleaning person, Subject A1, took anti-psychotic medication which caused frequent pacing in the experimental chamber, his studio apartment. He is also the researcher and author of this report. He persistently escaped the aversion of miniscule particles adhering to his unclad feet by lifting his leg and brushing them off into a waste paper basket. ABAB conditions alternated among a clad-foot, non-cleaning, baseline measurement and a return to baseline condition and two unclad-foot-to-floor experimental cleaning conditions. The independent variable, A1's feet, varied between clad and unclad states. A1's mother, A2, measured for the dependent variable, floor cleanliness, by donning fresh white cotton gloves and swiping each finger across various locations on the floor. Relative floor dirtiness was determined by her cleanliness probes. A1 photographed the gloves and image processing software converted each black and white facsimile into a dirtiness percentage, grayness, total black pixels divided by total black and non-black pixels. The data revealed that the floor was much cleaner when he wore nothing on his feet.

Introduction

Housekeeping is a chore. Human energy levels are depleted by sweeping, vacuuming, and damp mopping a wooden floor. Noisy vacuums disturb the neighbors. Liquid cleaning chemicals are emptied into the water system and equipment is costly to replace.

This study shows that the human foot is an easier, quieter, more economical, and more environmental alternative to conventional cleaning methods. To date, peer-reviewed journals in Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) have no written reports specifically addressing the function of human body parts as cleaning apparati.

Further research is needed on the untapped potential of other appendices, such as 1) the lips and tongue for removing a layer of peanut butter from off of a stainless steel spoon which is resistant to soap-and-water peanut butter cleaning, 2) the finger nail rubbed over porcelain dishes being washed in the sink to remove stubborn, greasy gravy stains, 3) the easily-dried palm of the hand rubbed over the same clean, wet, dishes, combined with air drying them in a dish rack, in lieu of dampening and soiling a mold-spore-collecting kitchen towel, 4) the horizontally angled lower ridge of the forehead just below the eyebrows and just above the eye socket (used gently and carefully after no objection during an opthalmological consultation) to tightly secure to the skull the rim of a hat that keeps blowing off in the wind, and 5) the gap between the two front teeth for gripping an inserted strand of dental floss strung out over the lower lip and hanging down below the chin to free the fingers when the demand for another operation interrupts the flossing.

Participants

During the experiment, Subject A1 was a 45-year-old, 5 '8", apple-shaped, beefy, unpaid writer who had been living with his mother until he was 44. He is certificated and experienced as a "Teacher of the Handicapped" and he eventually became disabled from teaching. He had been diagnosed with a grandiose delusional disorder. He has been taking anti-psychotic medication since 1979 and his symptoms are mostly in remission. Besides weight gain, the only other major side effect of anti-psychotic medication has been has been chronically excessive and erratic sleeping patterns.

A2, A1's mother, was 68 at the time of the experiment. She is currently deceased. She had been a public elementary school teacher and reading specialist. She stood 5 '4" tall and presented a solid, stout, sturdy frame.

Both subjects wore glasses over their eyes.

Setting

The experiment took place in 2004 in the all-purpose room of A1's studio apartment which also contained a small bathroom and kitchen. The unit was one of four apartments in an old house with a wrap-around front porch set in a middle-class neighborhood near the boardwalk of a beach town at the New Jersey Shore.

The all-purpose-room floor was made of varnished oak slats. The kitchen floor was linoleum and the bathroom was tile.

Design

The research followed the ABAB, alternating condition protocol under the following conditions. On the last day of the each period, Subject A2, A1's mother, collected primary data on the dependent variable, the cleanliness of the floor. A1's computer software converted the measurement of each of her probes into a mathematical representation of the cleanliness.

Condition A-1: In this pre-experimental, baseline measurement condition, A1's feet were almost always clad and he hadn't cleaned the floor with a mop, sponge, broom, vacuum, or any other conventional cleaning product for at least two months before A2 measured for cleanliness. Baseline cleanliness measurement: A-1: Day 1, the only day of A-1. Independent variable: clad feet.

Condition B-1: A1 lifted floor particles from off the floor and brushed them into a knee-high garbage pail with consistent, persistent multitudinous trials when he wore nothing on his feet. A2's cleanliness probe: B-1: Day 14, the final day of B-1. Independent variable: unclad feet.

Condition A-2: This return to baseline, control condition lasted for three months and thirteen days. A1's housecleaning, including by foot, had ceased almost entirely. When she returned, A2 probed for cleanliness on A-2: Day 103, the final day of A-2. Independent variable: clad feet.

Condition B-2:This was the return experiment condition. Cleanliness probe: B-2: Day 14, the final day of B-2. Independent variable: unclad feet.

Method

This study began as real world interactions between David, the resident, Cornelia, his mother. Cornelia visited David's apartment on four occasions and encouraged him to clean his floor. Since she was a teacher as well as a mother she had ample behavior modification experience.

During each visit, she wore a white cotton glove and swiped it across the floor. She showed him the grime she had lifted from off the floor and encourage him to clean it. During the visits, he kept a diary and she gave him each glove she used to inspect the floor.

When her first visit revealed a dirty floor, David resolved to clean it and keep it clean so he developed a self-behavior modification barefoot-to-floor cleaning plan. He kept a diary of his progress.

Four times Cornelia visited and inspected the floor. She gave him all four of the gloves she had used.

He saved the gloves and the diary and analyzed the data after Cornelia had passed away peacefully from inside a room at a local hospital.

One year after she passed away, A1 analyzed the old gloves he had saved from each of her four inspections and the diary he had kept of his cleaning progress.

He set the baseline glove on top of a white sheet of paper, photographed the image in black and white, and uploaded it to his laptop. Its image processing software determined the percentage of gray on the glove, total black pixels divided by total black and non-black pixels. The percentage numeral rendered relative floor dirtiness, into a measurable, manipulable dependent variable. Clad feet versus unclad feet became the independent variable.

He believed that the intensity and direction (favorable or unfavorable) of her vocal reaction to the dirt on the floor was a function of the level of gray on the glove.

Tiny pebbles, grains of sand, dirt, dust bunnies, cookie crumbs, and broken potato chip pieces adhere to unclad soles of bilaterally symmetric organisms when they ambulate across wooden and linoleum surfaces. Mechanoreceptors respond to mechanical cutaneous pressure and distortion when particles adhere to the glabrous epidermis of the anatomical structure. Afferent neurons carry the nerve impulses to the central nervous system which mobilizes musculoskeletal responses.

Therefore, the hypothesis of this experiment emerged as follows: Persistent, continuous, and multitudinous barefoot-to-floor cleaning trials by a home-dweller, during elongated periods of habitation, will noticeably clean a varnished wooden floor. One measurement of a grime-free residence is the relative level of discoloration on a white cotton glove that has been swiped by the fingers inside of it across various locations of a varnished wood floor.

A-1: Clad-Foot, Unclean Floor, Baseline-Recording, Pre-Experiment Condition

Under baseline Condition A-1, Subject A1 wore socks, slippers, or shoes without going barefoot, except while showering, dressing, and undressing, for a minimum of two months while home in his apartment. Then one day, quite by surprise A2 entered the scene knocking at the front door. A1 invited A2 in and she said, "You haven't seen me in a month."

He said, "I'm sorry, but I've been writing a lot."

She said, "You haven't been making your bed or have you?"

He said, "Mother, if I made the bed every morning, as soon as I napped, I'd mess it up again. That's what a TV news anchor told Barbara Walters when she asked him why men don't make their beds."

Then she pulled a fresh white cotton glove over her left hand, bent down to her knees, swiped the floor with her index finger, stood up, repositioned herself to another location on the floor, and swiped it again with her middle finger. then she did the ring finger, the pinky, and the thumb across different spots on the floor.

She said, "David, you should be ashamed of yourself. Look at this filth!" Then she pounced up on her feet, walked out of the apartment, and shut the door behind her. She went out to her car, retrieved a bag of white gloves, opened the door, this time without knocking, pounced back inside, set the bag on top of his laptop, removed her dirty white glove, and threw it on the floor.

She said, "Stop writing about other people's behavior and analyze your own for a change!"

Then she walked out the door and departed the scene.

A1 lifted the glove up from the floor and studied it intently.

Udderly humiliated A1 resolved to never let it happen again, but he didn't like sweeping, vacuuming, or mopping, so he was hit with the hunch that he could make his mother happy by cleaning the floor with his feet. (See Freud, 1960.)

He wondered if the filth on the glove triggered in her an emotional disgust reaction. He supposed she would come back soon with another white glove and keep running her fingers over the floor.

B-1: Unclad-Foot-to-Floor-Cleaning Experiment Condition

Two weeks had passed since A-1: Day 1 while every day A paced in his bare naked feet. As expected, A2 entered exactly as expected on B-1: Day 14.

She said, "David, it smells nice in here. What have you done with your place?"

"I've been cleaning the floor, Mother."

"You have? What a good son! If you don't mind, I'm just going to see for myself."

"If you must, go ahead and make yourself happy."

Five times she dropped to her hands and knees and tested the floor. She inspected the glove and said, "David, you make me so happy. I knew you could do it!"

Then she stayed with her son for two hours when she emptied the icebox and cooked him some borscht.

David asked for the glove so the give him the glove and a big, fat hug and went home smiling from from ear to ear.

A-2: Clad-Foot, Non-Cleaning, Control Condition

On the first day of Condition A-2, Cornelia went away to France for her annual winter sabbatical. The house was poorly insulated. The temperature in the apartment fell. David donned slippers to keep his feet warm. While she was away, the potential threat of the conditioned aversive stimuli, dirty gloves and parental reproach, was removed, so he stopped cleaning his home until after she had returned.

Although the spoken words were different in form, after she arrived at the end of A-2, the function of their verbal behavior proceeded with only minor functional differences from the scene in A-1. This time, for example, instead of throwing the dirty glove on the floor, she tossed it at his face.

B-2: Return to Unclad-Foot-to-Floor-Cleaning Experiment Condition

B-2 was similar in form and function to B-1. This time David said to his mother, "I would like to save our glove as a memento of our reunification."

Results

Subject A2 swiped all five of her glove-inserted fingers across twenty different locations of A1's wooden floor during four separate conditions of an ABAB double-return-to-baseline experiment with two clad-feet, non-cleaning conditions (A-1 and A-2) and two subsequent unclad feet, barefoot-to-floor cleaning conditions (B-1 and B-2), as described in the methods section of this report. Mathematical representations of the amount of dirt on the floor on the last day of each condition revealed the floor was cleaner when he kept his shoes, socks and slippers off his feet, paced around the room, and brushed off the particles that stuck to his feet into the garage pail and into the latrine. The hypothesis, that persistent, continuous, and multitudinous barefoot-to-floor cleaning trials by a home-dweller, during elongated periods of habitation, will noticeably clean a varnished wooden floor, is therefore temporarily confirmed, pending corroboration or falsification by additional behavior analytic experiments.

Discussion (Being revised by the blogger)

In its first test, the hypothesis has been confirmed. More corroborative study is necessary to determine whether or not it fits within the framework of the theoretical ABA system.

Post experimental trials have determined that the foot can reach floor locations where brooms cannot. Avoidance of mop-wringing and vacuum maintenance occurs. Delaying equipment replacement creates remunerative generalized conditioned positive reinforcement, tacted in Standard English as "money-saving" or in the U.S.A. slang vernacular as "being a tightwad."

Facile responses replace the inescapably painstaking exertion involved in the conventional methods thus rendering the method efficient and effective.

Follow-up anecdotal evidence shows that David has maintained perpetual foot cleaning responses consistently for fifteen years after they performed the initial experiment. David and Cornelia lived happily and cooperatively in nearby towns for fourteen year until she peacefully passed away from inside a room at a local hospital.

Members of the genus Homo should ask physicians about heath hazards before undertaking a foot-to-floor cleaning plan, wash their hands and feet before and after each operation, and take common sense safety precautions such as sweeping and vacuuming a floor after glass breakage and doffing shoes while penetrating an abode.

This experimenter's altruistic behavior is well-maintained by infrequent reinforcement schedules, so he will submit reports to open-access, peer-reviewed ABA journals instead of submitting the Human Foot to the U.S. Patent Office, so that fellow citizens of the international community can capitalize freely upon the findings.

Reference

Freud, S. (1960). Jokes and their relation to the unconscious (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York: W. W. Norton. (Original work published 1905)


Wednesday, October 15, 2014

Conclusions from a Seven-Year Inquiry into the Science and Ethics of Applied Behavior Analysis

This web log is about the ethics of behavior modification, a topic which includes issues pertinent to the professional practice of the science of human behavior known as Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA). After seven years of investigation into the science and ethics of ABA, as of today, this blogger is prepared to present all his general conclusions to date. Most of them are contained within the body of this paper.

Some Science of ABA

First of all, in a nutshell, ABA is mostly all about adding and removing various "schedules" of rewards and punishments contingent upon the emission of "target behaviors" in order to develop certain desirable behaviors in others or in one's self and in order to prevent or reduce certain "problem behaviors." Board Certified Behavior Analysts (BCBAs) know how to increase and decrease the frequency of occurrence of the specific behaviors they intend to modify. They also know how to extinguish behaviors, to make them disappear almost completely from the overall behavioral repertoires of human and non-human "organisms" alike. (See Pierce and Cheney, 2004.)

To their credit, they have discovered the alternatives to punishment. As though magicians cleverly twisting their positive techniques, they can make problem behaviors vanish into thin air while sparing the rod completely. BCBAs reinforce not-behaving ("badly"), they reward incompatible behaviors that cannot happen at the same time and place as unwanted behaviors, and they shape and reward lower and lower rates of responding of the behaviors they are paid to diminish by the parents, schools, and insurance companies who hire them. They establish positive instructional control by telling children to do something rewarding that they are already inclined to do and then later, when they teach them a skill that may tax them somewhat, they break it down into less taxing chunks, and rely on the pattern of instructional compliance previously habituated through positives to teach these kids some valuable skills. Under these conditions students do not try to avoid or escape challenging demands, because the consent of the child or incapacitated adult is automatic under total reinforcement. Punishment and noncompliance are non-issues when punishment is not necessary. (See Lavigna and Donnellan, 1986.)

Behavior analysts have also been known to punish self-injurious behavior, such as self-head-banging by people with severe disabilities, when they don't have enough positive methods or know-how at their disposal. They invented a device called The Self-Injurious Behavior Inhibiting System (SIBIS) which detects blows to the head and administers electric shock contingent upon the head-blowing behavior. When their work is ethically scrutinized, one can say that if and when punishment or coercion is temporarily and absolutely unavoidable to prevent, diminish, or eliminate seriously harmful rates of behavior, then the work of the analysts should never be considered satisfactory. They must return to the drawing board and try and try again until they find or create acceptable positive alternatives to punishment. (See Iwata, 1988.)

The Reputation and Promise of ABA

ABA has a bad public rap, however. Some of its reputation is warranted and most of it isn't. ABA is powerful. Power corrupts. The practice in the field can easily cross the line between helping and harming.

This blogger is helping them become more ethical so the public will more readily accept their work and so then we can move on to heal the world with broad-scale, ethical applications of human science. Since behavior analysts are good at changing the behaviors of people with less power, this blog challenges them to accept some modification of their own behaviors. Many of them appreciate it and many of them don't want to hear it, but what they dish out to others, they should be able to take in stride when they're treated with a dose of their own medicine. A bitter pill is hard to swallow, but good medicine is meant to heal.

I firmly believe that the environmental movement would benefit immensely if they knew how to use ABA to teach people how to reduce their carbon footprints by stressing the money savings quality of conservation and lightening up on its constant bombardment of doom and gloom scenarios off into the future and if they knew enough how to modify the behaviors of politicians with ample reinforcement, massive amounts of popular praise, for the things they do well. As seen in a count of BUYcott app users who sign up for campaigns to either reward or to penalize corporate behavior, more people prefer to penalize companies than reward them, which is a trend I might extrapolate to the possible existence of an aggregated public preference for punishment strategies over reward strategies. I have no doubt that an intense collaboration among greens and behavior analysts would stem the tide of global warming, but this will not happen if the ABA profession-at-large does not improve its ethics.

Some Ethics of ABA

Science tells BCBAs how to change human behavior, but only ethical analyses can prescribe which behaviors ought to change, so ABA is no good without ethics. BCBA training does include courses in ethics, but ABA in practice has a good share of ethical problems.

Since its inception in 1968, their flagship "Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis" (JABA) has been identifying "problems of social importance" as targets for change. For example, while gay pride marches in New York City first began to commemorate the 1969 Stonewall Riot that sparked the modern gay pride movement, some JABA articles were also labeling gay sexual activity as aberrant behavior. The gay liberation movement had to fight long and hard battles to remove homosexuality from psychology's lists of "disorders."

ABA today targets for reduction certain autistic behaviors. Indeed, ABA "treatment" of autism has spawned into a major industry. Many atypical autistic behaviors such as body-rocking and hand-flapping cause no physical harm to people or property, but parents will pay BCBA's to squelch these behaviors because they're afraid they won't "fit in" with their peers. Meanwhile opposing communities of autistic adults are comparing ABA's history with gays to the current events involving autistic children. The leaders are saying, "Don't try to change us. Changing us hurts us. We are not a disease. We are proud of our autistic identity. Just change society so they accept us and embrace us for the atypical people we happen to be." Google "Neurodiversity" to witness the birth of a new movement.

Furthermore, young autistic children are virtually powerless on their own to stop behavior change plans once they're set into motion. Theoretically, the Behavior Analysis Certification Board (BACB) does recommend that their BCBAs seek the dual consent of both the parent or guardian and the child or incapacitated adult when they implement steps toward behavior "control," as B. F. Skinner called it. During practice, however, they have not been known to grant the recipients of their treatments the power to dissent from punishment when their surrogate decision makers had already signed legally-written informed consent documents. Child study teams may give them a seat at the table during behavior plan meetings and they may "cooperate" there out of fear of reprisals, not knowing what they're getting into, but under the terms of informed consent, young children and incapacitated adults have no authority to object to what's been approved, especially when their parent advocates are not watching the actual events unfold. (If parents do not observe the "treatments" taking place, then with the consent of their children, they could insist their BCBA contractors audio-visually film all the activities.)

BCBAs may say they are looking out for the "welfare" of the children when they punish them or reward them and most of them emphasize a predominance of skills training under positively reinforcing contingencies, but the denial of dissent when punishment does occur obstructs the free exercise of the right to self-determination, the equitable right to autonomy that everybody shares, regardless of age or cognitive ability. Even when they do consider the best-interest of the child, well-meaning parents and analysts can and do make harmful mistakes. If they listened more to the children, problems would be prevented. Taking us beyond the simple ABA-profession's notion of welfare without autonomy, nuances can be argued such that the autonomy of the child is vital to the best-interest of the child and the exercise of one right does not block the other.

To illustrate the ABA paucity of partners in consent, consider the most egregious case known in the press, where the staff at the Judge Rotenberg Center (JRC), near Boston, MA, present painful electric skin shock to autistic children and others soon after they "emit" behaviors as mild and harmless as not doing as they're told. When it was presented in a lawsuit against JRC, Fox News recorded the videotape evidence of JRC employees restraining a disabled boy face down upon a board and shocking him while he was "screaming for help." Warning: the footage is graphic. JRC shocked him 31 times that day. Fox News said they shocked him for "refusing to take off his coat." JRC called it "treatment." The boy's mother called it terror and abuse. She said to Fox News that when she signed him into the school, she "had no idea, no idea, that they tortured the children in the school." Fox News said she had testified to the jury about watching the video and hearing staff members laughing while her son was on the floor. "She got him to the hospital that day where he was diagnosed with acute stress response caused by the shocks." She told Fox news she tried to turn his head that day but it only stayed straight. When she waived her hand in front of his face, his eyes didn't blink.

The United Nations also called it torture. The Food and Drug Administration is currently deliberating on its upcoming decision on whether or not to ban JRC's patented Contingent Skin Shock (CSS) devices. The issue is still reoccurring in the mainstream media.

One day in the course of my study, I was tweeting with an anonymous user of JRC's Twitter account. I proposed several challenging tweets and he or she tried to defend them all, until I sent my final question and there were no more replies. I was asking him or her if JRC board members, administrators, staff, outside supporters from within the ABA profession, and especially the founder of the institution, Dr. Matthew Israel, had ever subjected themselves to the identical contingencies they were using to torture their children. This would include being tied to a board by people with more power who are laughing at you when they let you stand and you fall down to the ground while they're shocking your skin with the same intensity they have already used on their children, after they told you to take off your coat, but you refused to do it knowing your coat-less body would make it easier for them to shock you again and again, so they shocked you for disobeying this command, as one more shock out of a total of 31 shocks in one day, rendering you completely into a stress-induced zombie.

The Golden Rule of Ethics (empathy) requires us to do unto others as we'd have them do unto us. Therefore, any of the JRC members or supporters as listed above who do not voluntarily submit themselves precisely to this specific contingency cannot legitimately describe their professional work as ethical. Also, BCBAs and other behaviorists and public officials who know this has happened and do nothing to stop it are complicit in the torture.

Most unfortunately for the children, but also for the reputation of the professional community of behavior analysts, the Association for Behavior Analysis International (ABAI) gives JRC leaders an audience of their peers when they present and defend their "aversive therapy" at ABAI convention "symposiums." Critics of ABA hit them hard with legitimate arguments, especially regarding their ties with JRC. Obviously, their reputation suffers, yet they continue to support JRC or try to ignore the JRC controversy. It is quite unfathomable to this blogger why all the ABA professional membership organizations don't issue public statements about the harmful nature of painful electric skin shock contingent upon "noncompliance," as the JRC leaders proudly described to their ABAI audience.

Here's a clue to a possible answer. CQ Weekly reported that in March alone of 2010, JRC paid "the lobbyist, Edward D. Krenik of Bracewell and Giuliani, an EPA liaison to Congress in the Bush administration," $20,000 to "persuade senators to provide an exemption — in legislation that would deny federal funding to schools that physically restrain students — for the court-approved electric shocks that the Judge Rotenberg Center uses with some of its students." (Zeller, 2010) When there's a problem, it pays to follow the money. So to what extend does JRC lobby the ABAI?

The Future of ABA

To conclude the conclusions of this long-term investigation, ABA is ethical, in general, to a degree, but it has much need for improvement. It is vulnerable to other solid criticisms not mentioned above, such as the threat of its enormous potential falling into the wrong hands of authoritarian rulers. If the profession would publicly tackle its lingering deficiencies and build further upon its strengths, its reputation would no doubt improve. Then the world can move on and heal itself through the scientific design of culture, as the founder of their science, B.F. Skinner, had envisioned. This can only happen in a good way, however, when it's extremely well restrained.

If the ABA profession does not want to improve its ethical status and then involve themselves in cultural issues, then society needs to proceed without them and apply the principles of Skinnerean science without the aid of those individuals whose salaries depend on the control of autistic behavior, for instance.

As my psychologist said to me last week, Joseph Stalin had amassed nearly absolute power over the Soviet Union. Among his many atrocities, his followers executed about 250,000 people of foreign ethnicities. (From Wikipedia, Joseph Stalin, Purges and Deportations referencing Montefiore (2004).) While Stalin was corrupting the theories of Marx, French intellectuals on the left, supporters of communism such as the existentialist philosopher Jean Paul Sartre, did not break away from Soviet ideology. So what will become of the theories of behaviorism, my doctor wondered, if its own leading intellectuals refuse to denounce the JRC?*

Post Script

As a post script here, the most viewed essay in this blog describes at length how parents, teachers, and BCBAs can obtain the dual consent of the recipients of their behavior change programs along with their parents or legal guardians, if they have them.

Footnote

* October 16, 2014 Google Scholar searches of the exact phrase: "Judge Rotenberg Center" in leading ABA journals returned no articles. Searches included the Behavior Analysts, published by ABAI, JABA, published by Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, and Behavior and Philosophy, by the Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies, Behavior Analysis Digest, Behavior Analyst Online, etc.

Reference List (being compiled)


Zeller, Shawn. "Client of the Month: The Judge Rotenberg Center." CQ Weekly (July 12, 2010): 1675. http://library.cqpress.com/cqweekly/weeklyreport111-000003698961

Montefiore, Simon Sebag (2004). Stalin: The Court of the Red Tsar. ISBN 0-7538-1766-7


Copyright and Disclaimers

Reward and Consent , © is January 15, 2007 to the current date. All rights reserved (and stuff like that). E-mail me for permission to reproduce in part or in full. Please link to and cite passages quoted or paraphrased from here.

Reward and Con
sent is not responsible for links on the site. For example, I use keywords "Operant Conditioning" in the YouTube search field for the videos displayed below the archives on the left. Google selects the videos and the results change from time to time. Please email me if anything is not educational and germane to the subject and I will reevaluate the search.

I am an advocate for people with disabilities certified to teach special education with a Master of Arts in Teaching. I am not a Licensed Psychologist or a Board Certified Behavior Analyst. When in doubt, seek the advice of an MD, a PhD, or a BCBA. My ability to analyze the ethics of ABA stems from the fact that I am disabled and ABA interventions are often done to people like me, which I voluntarily accept, but only when I alone am the person granting consent, and not a parent, sibling, guardian, or institution.